Difference between revisions of "Criticism of Mormonism/Cognitive dissonance"

m (What is cognitive dissonance?)
m
 
(47 intermediate revisions by 7 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{Articles FAIR copyright}} {{Articles Header 1}} {{Articles Header 2}} {{Articles Header 3}} {{Articles Header 4}} {{Articles Header 5}} {{Articles Header 6}} {{Articles Header 7}} {{Articles Header 8}} {{Articles Header 9}} {{Articles Header 10}}
+
{{Main Page}}  
=={{Criticism label}}==
+
{{H2
Many critics of the Church portray members as either naive, ill-informed dupes or cynical exploiters. Fortunately, most fair-minded people realize that—just as in any religion—there are many intelligent, well-informed people who become or remain members of the Church.  To get around this, some critics appeal to the psychological concept of 'cognitive dissonance' to dismiss the spiritual witnesses of intelligent, articulate members.
+
|L=Criticism of Mormonism/Cognitive dissonance
 +
|H=Mormonism and "cognitive dissonance"
 +
|S=
 +
|L1=Question: What is "cognitive dissonance" and how does it relate to Mormonism?
 +
|L2=Wendy Ulrich, Ph.D., "“Believest thou…?”: Faith, Cognitive Dissonance, and the Psychology of Religious Experience"
 +
}}
 +
{{:Question: What is "cognitive dissonance" and how does it relate to Mormonism?}}
  
{{CriticalSources}}
+
{{PerspectivesBar
 +
|link=http://www.fairmormon.org/perspectives/fair-conferences/2005-fair-conference/2005-believest-thou-faith-cognitive-dissonance-and-the-psychology-of-religious-experience
 +
|author=Wendy Ulrich, Ph.D.
 +
|authorlink=http://www.fairmormon.org/perspectives/authors/ulrich-wendy
 +
|title=“Believest thou…?”: Faith, Cognitive Dissonance, and the Psychology of Religious Experience
 +
|publication=Proceedings of the 2005 FAIR Conference
 +
|date=August 2005
 +
|summary=It’s an old and frequent spiritual question, and it shows up in many forms. It is the question Jesus asks the disciples who hear his troubling and offending discourse on being someone whose flesh must be eaten and whose blood drunk by those who would have eternal life. The discourse confuses many, who turn back and follow him no more, and then, to those who remain Jesus asks the question, “Will ye also go away?”1 To the man who seeks out Jesus to heal his deeply troubled son, the question is implied, “If thou canst believe, all things are possible to him that believeth.”2 To Nephi, approached by an angel after he is carried away to the top of a high mountain, the question is more direct: “Believest thou that thy father saw the tree of which he hath spoken?”3 And to the brother of Jared, who asks to see the premortal Jesus after hearing his voice and seeing his hand, the question is perhaps most clearly stated, “Believest thou the words which I shall speak?”
 +
}}
  
=={{Conclusion label}}==
+
{{To learn more box:cognitive dissonance}}
"Cognitive dissonance theory," when applied with the critics' approach to explaining away the witnesses and convictions of others, is hardly scientific.  Critics' efforts fail on many grounds:
 
  
* critics begin by deciding what sorts of evidence or beliefs are "rational" and which are "false" or "irrational."  In doing so, the dice are loaded from the start, since it's the critics' epistomological assumptions that control the outcome.
+
</onlyinclude>
* critics can explain and dismiss any attitude, any belief, or any conviction
+
{{endnotes sources}}
* critics rely on claims about hidden, unverifiable, “subconscious” motivations as explanations
 
* critics assume they, as interpreters, know more about the subjects' experiences and inner states than the subjects themselves.  Such claims remain stable even if the subjects dispute the analysis.
 
  
Also problematic is are critics' refusals to acknowledge their own vulnerability to the same kind of misapplication of cognitive dissonance theory.  Any argument which critics can use against believing Church members can be reversed and used against critics in turn since whoever establishes the ground rules for what is rational or "true" controls the outcome of the analysis.
 
  
== ==
 
{{Response label}}
 
===What is cognitive dissonance?===
 
Cognitive dissonance theory was first described in the mid 1950s by Leon Festinger.
 
  
Cognitive dissonance explains behavior by pointing out that all people have various beliefs, thoughts, or ideas, called "cognitions."  From time to time, these cognitions will come into conflict &mdash;for example, someone might believe their child is honest and law-abiding.  However, they might learn one day that their child has been charged with shoplifting.  There are now two cognitions in tension:
 
*cognition #1: "my child is honest"
 
*cognition #2: "my child has been arrested for shoplifting"
 
  
These cognitions create conflict, or "dissonance" because they create internal conflict&mdash;it is not readily apparent how both cognitions can be 'true'.  This realization is a psychologically unpleasant experience.  According to the theory, people seek to minimize or resolve dissonance.  This can be done in a number of ways:
 
;the former cognition can be rejected:"I guess my child isn't as honest as I thought he was."
 
;the new cognition can be rejected:"My child wouldn't take something without paying.  There must be a mistake." ''or''  "It's a lie!  He was framed!"
 
;a new cognition can eventually be formed which reconciles the two conflicting cognitions:"My child put something in his shopping cart, and forgot to pay for it on leaving the store.  Thus, he was not trying to be dishonest, but it is understandable why he was arrested.  It was a misunderstanding."
 
  
The important point is that all people experience cognitive dissonance whenever they encounter something that does not match what they have previously thought or believed.  People may choose appropriate means of reconciling their dissonance (e.g. accepting new truths, adopting new perspectives, rejecting or modifying previous beliefs) or less appropriate ones (e.g. denying new truths, clinging to false ideas). 
+
<!-- PLEASE DO NOT REMOVE ANYTHING BELOW THIS LINE -->
 
 
The operation of cognitive dissonance alone says nothing about the quality or truth of someone's beliefs.  For example, in the third case, the child might truly have forgotten to pay for the article, or the parent might have seized on a rather threadbare excuse (not bothering to ask, "How did you ''forget'' the radio was hidden under your jacket?") and accepted it uncritically, because rejecting the first cognition&mdash;my child is honest&mdash;is too painful.  The presence, or resolution, of dissonance proves nothing about the facts.
 
 
 
===How do the critics misuse it?===
 
 
 
Michael Shermer, an agnostic and writer for ''Skeptic'' magazine, specifically dismissed the idea that "cognitive dissonance" could serve as a tool to explain away the convictions of religious believers as a group:
 
 
 
:It would be a long stretch to classify [millions of white, middle class American Christians] as oppressed, disenfranchised, or marginalized…[millions of apocalyptically-inclined] Americans are anything but in a state of learned helplessness or cognitive dissonance.  Indeed, some recent polls and studies indicate that religious people, on average, may be both physically and psychologically happier and healthier than non-believers.{{ref|shermer1}}
 
 
 
This is not to say that Shermer would deny that ''some'' believers might be the victims of cognitive dissonance.  However, an attempt to use cognitive dissonance to explain an entire group of believers is too unscientific a stance for Shermer to embrace.
 
 
 
Critics like to pretend that their use of 'cognitive dissonance' is very scientific, and objective.  However, they usually ignore one of the most important principles of a scientific explanation: falsifiability.
 
 
 
:The criterion of falsifiability...says that statements or systems of statements, in order to be ranked as scientific, must be capable of conflicting with possible, or conceivable, observations.{{ref|popper1}}
 
 
 
The hallmark of pseudoscience is its inability to be falsified.  That is why neither religion or any other philosophical system can ever be called science, or tested by science.  “God made it all out of nothing in seven days, and faked the evidence,” says the young earth creationist.  “Any Mormon who doesn’t interpret the evidence as I do must be suffering cognitive dissonance,” says the anti-Mormon.
 
 
 
How could a faithful Mormon's behavior or attitude toward the evidence prove that he or she is not subject to the critics' "cognitive dissonance"?
 
 
 
There is nothing which the critic could not shoe-horn into his theory&mdash;cognitive dissonance is thus little but a handy club to beat anyone who does not share his interpretation.  “Of course you see it differently,“ the critic can kindly, but oh-so-condescendingly assure his Mormon friend.  “You’re still in the grip of cognitive dissonance.”
 
 
 
The anti-Mormon (ab)use of the theory is especially vulnerable to the charge of being unfalsifiable, but a lack of falsifiability has long been the chief criticism of cognitive dissonance theory generally:
 
 
 
:One continuous criticism of Dr. Festinger's theory is that is may not be falsifiable.  That is, there is no solid empirical data that proves without a doubt that people will react in a specific manner in a given situation or when dealing with dissonance.{{ref|web1}}
 
 
 
Dissonance is easier to point to when a group of people is exposed to the same situation and choices under controlled conditions.  Trying to tease out why a given individual holds to or rejects specific religious or philosophical positions is a much taller order.  There are no controls on the critics' rampant speculation, since they often wave the idea of "cognitive dissonance" about without having studied a particular believer under controlled conditions (or even without having spoken with the person they are 'diagnosing').
 
 
 
===Is turnabout fair play?===
 
 
 
This is not to say that cognitive dissonance cannot play a role in religious belief.  It plays a potential roles in beliefs of ''all sorts''&mdash;the religious as well as the a-religious.
 
 
 
It might play a role in some Mormons' refusal to accept an uncomfortable truth.  It could also play a role in the critics' experiences, in which their expectations and beliefs did not meet their perceptions of reality.  Each critic is the only one able to make that assessment. 
 
 
 
But, lacking access to others’ reasoning and spiritual experiences, a critic cannot objectively judge the influence (if any) of cognitive dissonance in others’ decisions.  He can worry about the dissonant [http://scriptures.lds.org/matt/7/3#5 beams] in his own eye; others’ [http://scriptures.lds.org/luke/6/41#42 motes] are out of the reach of his self-justifying inquiry.
 
 
 
Many critics seem unwilling to recognize that men and women of good will and sound intelligence might honestly disagree on the interpretation of evidence, even if considered with all the objectivity they can muster.  This is, for example, why some people will buy stock at a price at which other people are eager to sell.  (But perhaps the entire economy is merely an exercise in cognitive dissonance?)
 
 
 
LDS critics often have a naïve, super-simplified view of the historian’s work whereby anyone who disbelieves a religious account is somehow automatically more free from bias than a believer.  Such a stance ignores the fact that unbelievers may feel at least as great a stake in disproving uncomfortable and uncompromising religious claims as believers might in supporting them.  It is therefore no surprise that critics label interpretations with which they do not agree as examples of “cognitive dissonance” in action, while the critics' positions are portrayed as merely the product of dispassionate analysis.
 
 
 
One critic fond of this 'theory' tells us:
 
 
 
:The most important part of this analysis, by far, is to recognize that the forces we are about to discuss [cognitive dissonance] operate mostly at the subconscious level.  To the extent we drag them into the conscious realm, they largely stop operating.{{ref|mccue1}}
 
 
 
“Subconscious” forces which are used to explain behavior, especially by the outside observer, are a classic unfalsifiable hypothesis.  How can we know that a “cause” which has been supposedly dragged from subconscious to awareness is the genuine article?
 
 
 
Why isn’t our “discovered” reason simply a rationalization, which is driven in turn by an even deeper “subconscious force” and so on down forever?  Since a person is&mdash;by definition&mdash;unaware of unconscious processes, how can the critic know with any confidence that the "forces we are about to discuss" look anything like the unconscious ones? 
 
 
 
How can you say that A and B are the same thing if no one can get a certain look at A?
 
 
 
If this is difficult in oneself, how much harder is it in another person, to whose mind and experience the outsider has no direct access?  Despite these major hurdles, the critics seems to presume that they can reliably determine what others’ unconscious processes are and “drag them into the conscious realm.”  Freud would have been envious.
 
 
 
The critic then makes the equally strange assertion that these effects “largely stop operating” if we are but aware of them.  Even if the critic, by the greatest fortune, has indeed identified a proper “subconscious force”&mdash;something of which he can never be sure&mdash;this belief is extraordinarily optimistic.  Anyone who has spent any time in counseling or mental health work knows that awareness of a problem rarely provides a direct line to altered thinking or behavior.  If it did, therapy would be just a dump of information to the patient.
 
 
 
The critic goes on:
 
 
 
:The message that booms through the above evidence to me is that the denial inducing nature of cognitive dissonance makes it difficult to self-diagnose.{{ref|mccue2}}
 
 
 
Unfortunately for the critic, if we assume that this is true, then ''critics'' are equally vulnerable to the same treatment.  The Mormon could just as easily respond that an anti-Mormon's perspective is all due to cognitive dissonance.  He just doesn't know it, because such a condition is "difficult to self-diagnose."
 
 
 
This illustrates that whatever else might be said about the flaws in this misuse of cognitive dissonance theory&mdash;the lynch-pin (“most important part…by far”) of which is an unfalsifiable and unverifiable claim about subconscious motives&mdash;it is not rational and not scientific. 
 
 
 
But, appeals to "cognitive dissonance" allow the critic to fit the evidence to his biases, and “diagnose” flaws in others.  No matter how much his Mormon target might insist that the critic does not understand the Mormon's point of view or evaluation of the evidence, this just serves as stronger evidence to the critic of how deluded the Mormon is.  Cognitive dissonance in the critics' hands is nothing but [http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Self-fulfilling_prophecy self-fulfilling prophecy], or a variation of the [http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Observer-expectancy_effect observer-expectancy effect].  It is full of [[Cognitive_dissonance#Further reading | fallacies]], a substitute for rational discussion of the evidence and the witness of the Spirit.
 
 
 
=={{Endnotes label}}==
 
#{{note|shermer1}}Michael Shermer, ''How We Believe: The Search for God in an Age of Science'' (New York: WH Freeman and Company, 1999),211&ndash;212.
 
#{{note|popper1}}Karl Popper, ''Conjectures and Refutations'' (London: Routledge and Keagan Paul, 1963), 33.
 
#{{note|web1}}M. Bruce Abbot, “Cognitive Dissonance Theory,” class notes for ADV382J, University of Texas at Austin, September 2003 (accessed 31 October 2005). {{link|url=http://www.ciadvertising.org/sa/fall_03/adv382J/mbabbott/critique2.htm}}
 
#{{note|mccue1}}Bob McCue, “Notes for Van Hale’s Radio Show”; e-mail posting (5 September 2004), copy in author's possession.
 
#{{note|mccue2}}Bob McCue, “Notes for Van Hale’s Radio Show”; e-mail posting (5 September 2004), copy in author's possession.
 
 
 
{{FurtherReading}}
 
 
 
{{Articles Footer 1}} {{Articles Footer 2}} {{Articles Footer 3}} {{Articles Footer 4}} {{Articles Footer 5}} {{Articles Footer 6}} {{Articles Footer 7}} {{Articles Footer 8}} {{Articles Footer 9}} {{Articles Footer 10}}
 
 
 
[[fr:Cognitive dissonance]]
 

Latest revision as of 17:03, 22 April 2024

Mormonism and "cognitive dissonance"


Jump to details:

Question: What is "cognitive dissonance" and how does it relate to Mormonism?

Some critics of Mormonism appeal to the social psychological concept of “cognitive dissonance” to dismiss the spiritual witnesses of intelligent, articulate members

Many critics of the Church portray members as either naive, ill-informed dupes or cynical exploiters. Fortunately, most fair-minded people realize that—just as in any religion—there are intelligent, well-informed people who become or remain members of the Church. In response, some critics appeal to the social psychological concept of “cognitive dissonance” to dismiss the spiritual witnesses of intelligent, articulate members. [1]

Cognitive dissonance theory, when applied with a critic’s intent to explaining away the witnesses and convictions of believers, is badly flawed

Cognitive dissonance theory, when applied with a critic’s intent to explaining away the witnesses and convictions of believers, is badly flawed. Critics' efforts fail on many grounds:

  • Cognitive dissonance is a “motivational state” like hope or remorse or love. The fact that it may be operating does not exclude the possibility of a spiritual witness.
  • Critics define what evidence or beliefs are "rational" and which are "false" or "irrational." In doing so, the dice are loaded from the start since it's the critics' epistemological assumptions that will determine the outcome.
  • Critics can dismiss any attitude or conviction using the concept of cognitive dissonance. Conversely, believers can dismiss any argument made by critics using the same concept. The arguments cancel each other, resulting in a nil score.
  • Critics’ claims rely on inferences about hidden, unverifiable, unfalsifiable, “subconscious” sources.
  • Critics reject subjects' self-reports of experiences and inner states in favor of their own assumptions.

Critics argue that believers make claims of love for and faith in the gospel in order to explain why they would put up with the demands and difficulties of a Church oriented lifestyle

Cognitive dissonance theory was first described in the 1957 by social psychologist Leon Festinger.[2] The term describes the unpleasant feeling arising from an awareness of a difference between what we feel or believe and how we are actually acting. Cognitive dissonance can also arise when we hold two or more different beliefs that conflict with each other. It’s an uncomfortable, even painful state. It’s aversive enough to drive us to make changes. When we feel cognitive dissonance, we’re motivated to reduce it and return to a state of psychological harmony.

Critics argue that believers make claims of love for and faith in the gospel in order to explain why they would put up with the demands and difficulties of a Church oriented lifestyle. It’s argued by critics that members don’t actually receive spiritual witnesses. Instead, it’s argued, members invent personal spiritual myths to reduce the dissonance they feel when they take on religious rules and responsibilities that yield minimal rewards. Inventing a reward like a spiritual witness makes the dissonance go away. It makes the social transaction of compliance to a religious way of life make sense.

A Case Study

Consider the payment of tithing to the Church as a case study. If a Church member doesn’t pay tithing but believes he should be paying it, he’s in a state of cognitive dissonance. His beliefs are in conflict with his actions. It’s painful to him. In order to restore inner equilibrium, he can reduce the dissonance, acquire new information, or minimize the importance of the dissonance to a point where it doesn’t bother him anymore. The conflicted non-tithe payer can choose from four different strategies:

  1. Acquiring New Information—He might try to restore his inner harmony by gathering more information. Maybe he’ll comb news items to see how the Church spends money. He might demand an accounting from Church leaders detailing how all his individual donations are spent. He could continue to do this until he either a) decided tithing is well-spent and he should begin paying it or b) decided the Church is wasteful and/or misguided and doesn’t deserve his money until it undergoes a reformation. He might tell himself that he’d like to pay tithing but he can’t do it in good conscience when the Church is undeserving of the money.
  2. Minimizing the Importance of the Inconsistency—He might convince himself that it doesn’t matter that he doesn’t pay tithing because his tithing really isn’t important to the Church. After accepting this minimization, he can feel better about not paying it. After all, the Church appears prosperous. Its programs seem well-funded. It can afford to donate to humanitarian efforts. He could even promise himself if it ever looked like the Church was suffering and needed his personal funds, he’d start paying it.
  3. Reducing the Dissonance by Accepting the Attitude and Changing the Behavior—He might look for and emphasize the benefits of the behavior (paying tithing) and ignore the negative effects of giving up ten percent of one’s income. He pays tithing. He changes his behavior and eliminates the dissonance between his attitudes and actions. This is the process critics point to as acting as a counterfeit for conversion through a spiritual witness.
  4. Reducing the Dissonance by Accepting the Behavior and Changing the Attitude—In this case, the conflicted tithe payer accepts his non-payment of tithing. He brings his attitudes about tithing into harmony with his practice of not paying it. In order to do this, he now claims his former belief about the Church being “true” was wrong. His attitude has changed and he is justified in abandoning tithing and keeping his money.

How do the critics misuse it?

The Irony

This is where the irony of cognitive dissonance as a complaint against believers emerges. Of the four possibilities listed above, only one applies to people who persist in their belief in the Church. The other three can lead to a full or partial departure from Church life. Most of the strategies for managing cognitive dissonance don’t lead people to stay in the Church. On the contrary, they lead people out of it. The same kind of analysis could be made with other challenging aspects of Church life such as home teaching, sexual behavior, honesty, Sabbath observance, etc.

In the spirit of “live by the sword, die by the sword,” critics who level cognitive dissonance at believers should consider the role of the same phenomenon in their own thoughts, feelings, and convictions. All people experience cognitive dissonance when we do or learn something that does not match what we previously thought or believed. None of us is immune to it.

Even though cognitive dissonance may be real and universal, its operation alone says nothing about the quality or truth of someone's beliefs. The presence, or resolution, of dissonance proves nothing about the facts in question.

Motivational States

Admitting the possibility that cognitive dissonance may play a role in religious choices is not the same thing as dismissing the possibility that real spiritual witnesses are also factors. The dichotomy critics have used to frame the relationship between cognitive dissonance and a spiritual witness is a false one. Nothing in cognitive dissonance theory demands it be exclusive of all other motivating factors and influences.

From the very beginning, cognitive dissonance was a term meant to describe a “motivational state.” There are many different kinds of motivational states. They include our most basic feelings like hunger, fear and lust or they may be more complicated emotions like curiosity, guilt, hope, or love. Cognitive dissonance falls into the same category as these feelings. What they have in common is that they’re internal states that drive us to action.

Most of us will recognize motivational states such as hope and love and remorse as common themes in personal stories of spiritual witnesses. We’ve come to expect believers to refer to these kinds of motivational states in their accounts. Even Mormon scriptures deal at length with the role of hope and desire in faith acquisition Moroni 7:40 Alma 32:27. No one apologizes for the role of motivational states in spiritual life. The same frankness should exist when addressing the motivational state of cognitive dissonance. Why should believers be expected to assume a defensive posture when a critic complains cognitive dissonance is operating? It may be an underlying factor, like dozens of other possible factors. But that doesn’t mean it has to be the only one. That would be like claiming anything we do while we're hungry is the result of that hunger alone and can't be attributed to any other motivation. Such a claim would be clearly ridiculous.

Cognitive Dissonance Theory does not undermine the possibility of receiving a genuine spiritual witness. It simply illuminates another of many motivational states. These states may be tools or stepping stones believers can use as they progress toward more sublime experiences.

The Problem of Falsifiability

Michael Shermer, an agnostic and writer for Skeptic magazine, specifically dismissed the idea that cognitive dissonance could serve as a tool to explain away the convictions of religious believers as a group:

It would be a long stretch to classify [millions of white, middle class American Christians] as oppressed, disenfranchised, or marginalized…[millions of apocalyptically-inclined] Americans are anything but in a state of learned helplessness or cognitive dissonance. Indeed, some recent polls and studies indicate that religious people, on average, may be both physically and psychologically happier and healthier than non-believers.[3]

This is not to say Shermer would deny some believers might be misusing cognitive dissonance. However, he balks at using cognitive dissonance to explain an entire group of believers.

Non-believers claim that their invocation of cognitive dissonance is scientific and objective. However, this claim doesn't measure up to one of the most important principles of a scientific inquiry: falsifiability. And a hallmark of pseudoscience is its inability to be falsified.

The criterion of falsifiability...says that statements or systems of statements, in order to be ranked as scientific, must be capable of conflicting with possible, or conceivable, observations.[4]

It’s difficult to imagine a setting where faithful Mormons could be given a proper chance to prove that their resistance to anti-Mormon "evidence" operates independently of cognitive dissonance. Until someone can create such a setting, critics' claims about believers are not falsifiable and, therefore, not strictly scientific.

Dissonance is easier to identify when the group of people in question is exposed to the same situation and makes the same kinds of choices under controlled conditions. Festinger’s initial experiments on cognitive dissonance were conducted in controlled laboratory settings where simple tasks and questions were used to measure behaviors and attitudes. Though later attempts were made to extend the experimental principles into real social situations, no further claims about how cognitive dissonance actually operates can boast the same rigor as the basic experimental data. Trying to tease out why an individual holds or rejects specific religious or philosophical positions is a much taller order. Religious attitudes and behaviors are complicated and nuanced and drawn out over lifetimes. They don’t reduce well to laboratory settings. Critics try to extrapolate the first simple, straightforward findings into areas of social and spiritual life where the original concepts and methods can never venture.

Problems in the Rules of Engagement

This is not to say that cognitive dissonance cannot play a problematic role in religious beliefs. However, it can play a problematic role in beliefs of all sorts—the religious as well as the non-religious or even anti-religious.

It might play a role in some Mormons' refusal to accept uncomfortable “glitches.” But without access to others’ reasoning and spiritual experiences, critics cannot objectively evaluate the influence of cognitive dissonance in others’ religious attitudes and behaviors.

Many critics seem unwilling to recognize that men and women of good will and sound intelligence might honestly disagree on the interpretation of evidence, even when considering it with all the objectivity they can muster. This is, for example, why some people will buy stock at a price at which other people are eager to sell. Surely the entire economy isn’t merely an exercise in cognitive dissonance reduction.

LDS critics often have a naïve, over-simplified view of historians’ work. They may assume anyone who disbelieves a religious account is somehow more free from bias than a believer. Such a stance ignores the fact that unbelievers may feel as great a stake in disproving uncomfortable and uncompromising religious claims as believers might in supporting them. Non-believing critics may be prone to labeling interpretations with which they do not agree as examples of “cognitive dissonance” while their own positions are portrayed as products of dispassionate analysis.

Mining the Subconscious

One critic espousing cognitive dissonance as a problem in religious life tells us:

The most important part of this analysis, by far, is to recognize that the forces we are about to discuss [cognitive dissonance] operate mostly at the subconscious level. To the extent we drag them into the conscious realm, they largely stop operating.[5]

“Subconscious” forces used to explain behavior, especially by the outside observer, can only be unfalsifiable hypotheses. How can anyone know that a “cause” which has been supposedly dragged from subconscious is genuine? Since a person is—by definition—unaware of subconscious processes, how can the critic know with any confidence that the "forces we are about to discuss" look anything like the subconscious ones? How can anyone say that A and B are the same thing if no one can get a certain look at A?

Despite these major hurdles, critics seem to presume they can reliably determine others’ subconscious processes and “drag them into the conscious realm.” It’s an especially remarkable claim since critics don’t usually claim to have interviewed or analyzed any of the believers of whom they speak. Not even Freud made claims like these about our access to the subconscious.

The critic then makes the equally strange assertion that these effects “largely stop operating” if believers become aware of them. Even if the critic has identified a proper “subconscious force”—something of which he can never be sure—this belief is extraordinarily optimistic. The accepted wisdom in counseling or mental health work has long been that awareness of a problem rarely provides a direct line to altering thinking or behavior. Mental health services are not merely magic confessionals.

The critic goes on:

The message that booms through the above evidence to me is that the denial inducing nature of cognitive dissonance makes it difficult to self-diagnose.[6]

Unfortunately for the critics, if we assume that this is true, then critics themselves are equally vulnerable to the same treatment. The faithful Mormon could just as easily respond that an anti-Mormon's perspective is all due to cognitive dissonance. The anti-Mormon just doesn't know it because such a condition is "difficult to self-diagnose."

Appeals to cognitive dissonance allow the critic to fit the evidence to his biases and “diagnose” flaws in others. No matter how much faithful Mormons might insist the critic does not understand Mormons’ points of view or evaluations of the evidence, this just serves as stronger evidence to the critic of the depth of the Mormons’ delusions.

In the hands of critics, cognitive dissonance is a self-fulfilling prophecy, or a variation of the observer-expectancy effect. It is full of fallacies. It is a substitute for rational discussion of the evidence and for thoughtful consideration of the possibility of a real the witness of the Spirit.


Wendy Ulrich, Ph.D., "“Believest thou…?”: Faith, Cognitive Dissonance, and the Psychology of Religious Experience"

Wendy Ulrich, Ph.D.,  Proceedings of the 2005 FAIR Conference, (August 2005)
It’s an old and frequent spiritual question, and it shows up in many forms. It is the question Jesus asks the disciples who hear his troubling and offending discourse on being someone whose flesh must be eaten and whose blood drunk by those who would have eternal life. The discourse confuses many, who turn back and follow him no more, and then, to those who remain Jesus asks the question, “Will ye also go away?”1 To the man who seeks out Jesus to heal his deeply troubled son, the question is implied, “If thou canst believe, all things are possible to him that believeth.”2 To Nephi, approached by an angel after he is carried away to the top of a high mountain, the question is more direct: “Believest thou that thy father saw the tree of which he hath spoken?”3 And to the brother of Jared, who asks to see the premortal Jesus after hearing his voice and seeing his hand, the question is perhaps most clearly stated, “Believest thou the words which I shall speak?”

Click here to view the complete article

Learn more about cognitive dissonance
Key sources
  • Wendy Ulrich, "'Believest thou...?': Faith, Cognitive Dissonance, and the Psychology of Religious Experience," FAIR link
Wiki links
Online
  • Clark Goble, "Cognitive Dissonance and Confirmation Bias," Mormon Metaphysics blog post, (20 July 2007), libertypages.com. off-site
  • Cognitive dissonance - multiple links off-site
  • Critique of Cognitive Dissonance Theory off-site
  • Daniel C. Peterson and William J. Hamblin, "Is Spirituality All in Your Head?," MeridianMagazine.com off-site
  • Michael D. Jibson, "'Imagine: Review of The End of Faith: Religion, Terror, and the Future of Reason by Sam Harris'," FARMS Review 18/1 (2006). [233–264] link
  • Daniel C. Peterson, "Reflections on Secular Anti-Mormonism," FARMS Review 17/2 (2005). [423–450] link
  • Richard N. Williams, "The Spirit of Prophecy and the Spirit of Psychiatry: Restoration or Dissociation? (Review of The Sword of Laban: Joseph Smith Jr. and the Dissociated Mind)," FARMS Review (2000). [435–444] link
Video
"Believest thou?" Faith, Cognitive Dissonance, & Psychology of Religious Experience, Wendy Ulrich (Psychologist), 2005 FAIR Conference
Print
  • Leon Festinger, A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, 1957). ISBN 0804701318
  • Leon Festinger, Henry W. Riecken, and Stanley Schachter, When Prophecy Fails: a Social and Psychological Study of a Modern Group That Predicted the Destruction of the World, (Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press, 1956).


Notes

  1. Bob McCue, “Van Hale’s ‘Mormon Miscellaneous’ Radio Talk Show,” Version 3, 20 Sept 2004.
  2. Leon Festinger, A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance (Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson, 1957).
  3. Michael Shermer, How We Believe: The Search for God in an Age of Science (New York: WH Freeman and Company, 1999),211–212
  4. Karl Popper, Conjectures and Refutations (London: Routledge and Keagan Paul, 1963), 33.
  5. Bob McCue, “Notes for Van Hale’s Radio Show”; e-mail posting (5 September 2004).
  6. Bob McCue, “Notes for Van Hale’s Radio Show”; e-mail posting (5 September 2004).